Glyxambi
Home About us Editorial board Ahead of print Current issue Search Archives Submit article Instructions Subscribe Contacts Login 
  • Users Online: 1573
  • Home
  • Print this page
  • Email this page


 
   Table of Contents      
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2019  |  Volume : 67  |  Issue : 5  |  Page : 655-658

Comparison of torsional amplitudes between emmetropes and myopes using after-image slides


Department of Pediatric Ophthalmology, Institute of Ophthalmology, Joseph Eye Hospital, Trichy-1, Tamil Nadu, India

Date of Submission03-Oct-2018
Date of Acceptance08-Jan-2019
Date of Web Publication22-Apr-2019

Correspondence Address:
Dr. Antony Arokiadass Baskaran
Department of Pediatric Ophthalmology, Joseph Eye Hospital, Trichy - 1, Tamil Nadu
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/ijo.IJO_1663_18

Rights and Permissions
  Abstract 


Purpose: To describe the influence of corrected refractive error on measured torsional fusional amplitudes (TA) by comparing the TA between emmetropes and spectacle corrected myopes, using the after-image slides of the synoptophore, as targets. Methods: Fifty emmetropes (Group I) and 50 myopes (Group II) with best-corrected acuity of 6/6 in each eye were included in the study. Near point of convergence (NPC), near point of accommodation (NPA), and horizontal fusional amplitudes (HFA) were assessed in all the subjects. After-image slides, both horizontally aligned, were used as targets (without the bright flashes). One of the slides was rotated inwards, till cyclo-diplopia was reported by the subject; the procedure was repeated with the slide rotated outwards. The sum of the two readings was taken as TA. NPC, NPA, HFA, and TA were analyzed. Results: There was no significant difference in the NPC, NPA, and HFA between the two groups. The emmetropic subjects had significantly better torsional amplitude (8.4 ± 1.4 degrees) compared to myopes (7.7 ± 1.5 degrees, P = 0.03). We postulate that this difference may be due to perceived image minification, which brings the edges of retinal image of the targets closer to the fovea, thus rendering the myopes lesser tolerant to cyclodiplopia than emmetropes. Conclusion: Refractive error, corrected with spectacles, influences the measured TA. Myopic subjects have lesser torsional fusional amplitude than emmetropes.

Keywords: Cyclofusion, cyclotorsion, myopia, ocular torsion, torsional fusion amplitudes, torsional vergence amplitude


How to cite this article:
Baskaran AA, Britto T, Rajkumar S, Thomas PA, Jesudasan C A. Comparison of torsional amplitudes between emmetropes and myopes using after-image slides. Indian J Ophthalmol 2019;67:655-8

How to cite this URL:
Baskaran AA, Britto T, Rajkumar S, Thomas PA, Jesudasan C A. Comparison of torsional amplitudes between emmetropes and myopes using after-image slides. Indian J Ophthalmol [serial online] 2019 [cited 2019 May 22];67:655-8. Available from: http://www.ijo.in/text.asp?2019/67/5/655/256673



Fusion is the ability of the brain to form a single composite mental image from two similar images perceived separately by the two eyes each lacking in a small detail. Fusional vergence aids sensory fusion by maintaining the image of objects of interest at the fovea of both the eyes. Measurement of vergence amplitudes evaluates the ability of the motor system to overcome induced misalignment of the visual axis; horizontally (convergence, divergence), vertically (sursumvergence, deosursumvergence), and torsionally (incyclovergence, excyclovergence).

While horizontal and vertical misalignments can be induced by prisms, torsional vergence measurements necessitate the use of synoptophore or similar haploscopic devices. A variety of targets and devices have been employed in the demonstration of cyclofusion and assessment of cyclofusional amplitudes since the nineteenth century.[1] However, torsional fusional amplitudes or cyclofusional amplitudes are seldom measured in routine clinical practice. The influence of refractive errors on the cyclofusional amplitudes is not much known.

The present study was done to assess the influence of refractive errors on torsional fusion amplitudes using the horizontal apertures of the after-image slides as images on a synoptophore. We proposed to use the after-image slides of the synoptophore, both oriented in a horizontal fashion, as images, to compare the torsional fusional amplitudes of emmetropes and myopes.


  Methods Top


This descriptive, comparative study was done in a tertiary eye care hospital after approval by the Institutional Review Board. Patients aged 10 to 25 years and 6/6 BCVA were divided into 2 groups; Group I, emmetropes and Group II, myopes with refractive error > 1 diopter.

Patients above 25 years of age, those with previous history of ocular surgery or trauma, anterior or posterior segment pathology, manifest strabismus, no simultaneous perception, astigmatism of more than 0.5 cylinder, BCVA <6/6, anisometropia (difference of more than 1 D between the eyes), and/or any systemic disorders were excluded.

All patients underwent detailed evaluation including visual acuity assessment for near and distance, cover tests, extraocular motility assessment, orthoptic evaluation comprising measurement of near point of accommodation (NPA), and near point of convergence (NPC) with Royal Air Force (RAF) rule. Horizontal fusional amplitudes were measured with the synoptophore using the fusional slides.

The torsional fusional amplitude was measured in the following manner. The after-image slides [Figure 1] were placed in the slide holders, such that both were horizontally oriented. The torsional position of one of the slides was changed by intorting the synoptophore arm to the right side of the observer, while the arm to the left remained fixed, and the point at which the patient appreciated diplopia was noted. This procedure was then performed with the same synoptophore arm extorted. Torsional range was calculated as the sum of extorsion and intorsion amplitudes, beyond which the patients reported torsional diplopia. An average of three readings were taken for each patient. In the myopic group, all tests were done with the patients using appropriate spectacles.
Figure 1: Picture of after- image slides used in the study

Click here to view


During these tests, the after-image slides were used as targets alone and were not used with bright flash lights as done during a routine after image test.


  Results Top


One hundred patients (Group 1, 50 emmetropes and Group 2, 50 myopes) were included in the study. The demographic characteristics are summarized in [Table 1]. There was no statistically significant difference in the demographic characteristics between the groups.
Table 1: Age and gender characteristics of the study participants

Click here to view


The refractive error in the myopic group ranged from − 1 to − 7 D in either eye (mean RE: 2.75 ± 1.37 D; LE: 2.71 ± 1.41 D). In 44 subjects (88%) of this group, the refractive error was less than − 4 D. There was no significant difference in the refractive errors between the two eyes in the myopic group (paired t-test; t = 1.013; df = 49; P = 0.316).

The comparison of orthoptic parameters between both groups has been summarized in [Table 2]. There was no significant difference in the horizontal fusional amplitudes between the emmetropes and myopes. The torsional fusional amplitude was 8.38 ± 1.6 degrees in the emmetropes and 7.70 ± 1.5 degrees in the myopes [Table 2] and [Figure 2]; this difference was statistically significant.
Table 2: Comparison of orthoptic parameters between the emmetropes and myopes enrolled in the current study

Click here to view
Figure 2: Comparison of torsional amplitudes among emmetropes and myopes enrolled in the study

Click here to view


The negative association between the average of the refractive errors of either eye and torsional fusional amplitudes did not reach statistical significance (Pearson correlation, r = −1.4, P = 0.3).


  Discussion Top


Cyclorotation, which is the rotation of the eye around the anteroposterior axis, superior pole of the vertical tilting inwards in intorsion and outwards in extorsion, was described as early as 1868 by Ewald Hering.[2] The existence of “disjunctive cyclorotations” or “cyclofusion” – where the eyes perform simultaneous cyclorotations in opposite directions – was debated upon even then. Hering, and later many investigators, such as Kertesz, held the view that cyclofusion was predominantly sensory and that no motor cyclovergence occurred.[3] However, this was refuted by Crone[4],[5] and other investigators such as Hooten et al.,[6] who demonstrated the motor component of cyclofusion, using appropriate wide-field complex stimuli and objective methods.

Cyclofusion assumes importance in the management of patients with cyclovertical muscle palsies, especially superior oblique palsy. Patients with good cyclofusional amplitudes may be able to avoid diplopia without head tilt, especially in mild cyclovertical muscle palsies. When torsional disparity exceeds the cyclofusional range, binocular vision may be adversely affected.[7]

The measured cyclofusional amplitudes may vary with the type of stimulus, the dissociativeness of the tests and the status of the extraocular muscles.[8] As early as in 1946, Ogle propounded that the amplitude of ocular cyclorotation following the rotation of a target depends, to a large extent, on the details of the target.[1] Various targets have been used to induce and study cyclofusion. Nagel used horizontal lines in a stereoscope,[1] Hering used vertical lines[2] in each half of the stereoscope, Kertesz[3] used horizontal lines in a synoptophore to study cyclofusion, while Crone,[5] and Hooten et al.[6] advocated the use of wide-field complex stimuli to demonstrate the motor component of cyclofusion. Sen et al.[9] used specially-designed slides on the synoptophore, whereas Sharma et al.[8] used special slides on a stereo projector. Use of 3D Landolt E chart by random dot stereogram for measuring cyclofusional amplitudes has been reported by Heckmann et al.[10] After-image test was used as a tool of measuring cyclodeviation by Sood and Sen.[11] In one of this study, Horizontal contours were found to have a greater effect on cyclovergence compared to the vertical ones.[5] In this study, horizontally-oriented lines of the after-image slides were used as the targets to elicit cyclofusion. Wide-angle complex stimuli were shown to elicit the motor component better than simple and smaller stimuli. However, these images have to be specially constructed to be employed in this process. We aimed to bring out the utility of commonly available after-image slides with horizontal line images in measuring cyclofusion. Moreover, since we aimed to study the difference in cyclofusion between emmetropes and myopes, and because the same slides were used in both the groups to assess cyclofusion, the effect of stimulus on the difference may be considered to be minimal.

Subjective methods of assessing cyclofusion had been in vogue since the last century. However, objective methods were required to study the motor component of cyclofusion in detail. Kertesz[3] used a marked contact lens, Crone[5] used synchronized photographs to study the conjunctival vessels in detail and Hooten et al.[6] used photographs and a thread lying on the anesthetized cornea to study cyclofusion. Perimetric analysis of the blindspot using phase difference haploscope was used by Herzau,[12] a scleral suction contact ring connected to a shaft attached to a eddy current motor to study the mechanical stiffness was used by Simonz et al.[13] and a Kratz linear pointer with cordimeter was used by Paris.[14] Though being useful to delineate the motor component of cyclofusion, objective methods would involve the use of sophisticated instruments and technology. In this study, though the motor and sensory component of fusion could not be clearly demarcated, we opine that this demarcation serves little purpose to a clinician. Nevertheless, it was also pointed out by Crone et al.[5] that objective and subjective measurements correspond well with each other.

Differences in cyclofusional amplitudes among patients with different refractive errors have not been well-studied. Several studies[15],[16] suggest, that the fusional vergence and stimulus AC/A ratio measured with spectacle correction, before refractive surgery, may significantly differ after surgery. Thus, a patient with refractive error wearing optimal spectacle correction becomes technically different from an emmetrope, in the measurement of orthoptic parameters. The method of correction of the refractive error may have a role to play in this, though. In the current study, spectacle-corrected myopes were found to have significantly lower torsional fusional vergences than emmetropes [Table 2] and [Figure 2]. We postulate that this difference could be due to perceived minification of the images on the retina, rendering the edges of the horizontal lines closer to the fovea, thereby reducing tolerance to cyclodiplopia. Crone[5] and Guyton[17] have discussed the role of Panum's space, which becomes wider away from the fovea, in allowing sensory cyclofusion. Thus myopes, may have poorer tolerance to cyclodiploia, as compared to emmetropes, the peripheral Panum's space having greater tolerance to diplopia. We were not able to find conclusively whether the cyclofusional amplitudes decreased with increasing refractive error, since, the negative association between the value of the refractive error and the torsional range was statistically insignificant. This could probably be because most of the patients (88%) had a refractive error between 1 and 4 D and the fact that several optical, anatomical, physiological and perceptual factors play a role in the perceived image size.[18]

[Table 3] summarizes the values of cyclofusional amplitudes recorded by various authors in the literature. The torsional amplitudes obtained in our study, that is, 8 deg (emmetropes) are akin to those reported by various authors who employed synoptophore based-line targets[3],[4],[5],[6],[7],[8],[9] as evidenced in [Table 3]. It is a well-accepted fact that in the measurement of cyclofusional amplitudes, varied results may be obtained.[19] Techniques which are more dissociative yield lesser fusional amplitudes, whereas more physiological tests tend to yield higher torsional fusional amplitudes.[8]
Table 3: Cyclofusional amplitudes calculated in the literature

Click here to view


The advantage of this study is that, to our knowledge, the after image slides of the synoptophore, which are commonly available, have not been used for measuring torsional amplitudes, before. These slides would measure the sensory cyclofusion alone compared to other objective tests employed by various authors which measure sensory and motor cyclofusion separately. A potential disadvantage of using these slides is that there would be no dissimilarity between the two slides, as is normally evident in the fusion slides of the synoptophore. Moreover, these slides, unless modified, cannot be used to measure cyclodeviation. Another disadvantage of the study is that the axial length, corneal curvature and back vertex power of the spectacles of the subjects have not been measured. Though the torsional fusion amplitudes differed only marginally between both the groups and hence of questionable clinical importance in this study population, the lesser cyclofusional amplitudes among myopes may be clinically significant in patients with high refractive errors corrected with spectacles.

The after-image slides have been found to measure the torsional vergence ampltiudes, providing values consistent with similar studies found in the literature. Further studies comparing the torsional amplitudes between hypermetropes and myopes, between subjects corrected with spectacles and contact lenses, and among those with varying degrees of refractive errors may throw light on the mechanisms leading to decreased torsional fusional amplitudes in myopes.


  Conclusion Top


After-image slides can be used to measure torsional amplitudes. Refractive errors, corrected with spectacles, influence measured torsional amplitudes.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
  References Top

1.
Ogle KN, Ellerbrock VJ. Cyclofusional movements. Arch Ophthalmol Chic Ill 1929. 1946;36:700-35.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Hering E. The Theory of Binocular Vision: Ewald Hering (1868). Springer Science and Business Media; 2012. p. 251.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Kertesz AE, Jones RW. Human cyclofusional response. Vision Res 1970;10:891-6.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Crone RA. Human cyclofusional response. Vision Res 1971;11:1357-8.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Crone RA, Everhard-Hard Y. Optically induced eye torsion. I. Fusion cyclovergence. Albrecht Von Graefes Arch Klin Exp Ophthalmol 1975;195:231-9.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Hooten K, Myers E, Worrall R, Stark L. Cyclovergence: The motor response to cyclodisparity. Albrecht Von Graefes Arch Klin Exp Ophthalmol 1979;210:65-8.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Georgievski Z, Sleep M, Koklanis K. Simulated torsional disparity disrupts horizontal fusion and stereopsis. J AAPOS Off Publ Am Assoc Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 2007;11:120-4.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Sharma P, Prasad K, Khokhar S. Cyclofusion in normal and superior oblique palsy subjects. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 1999;36:264-70.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Sen DK, Singh B, Mathur GP. Torsional fusional vergences and assessment of cyclodeviation by synoptophore method. Br J Ophthalmol 1980;64:354-7.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Heckmann J, Sturm V, Landau K, Török B. [Assessment of cyclofusion capabilities of normal subjects with 3D dynamic random-dot stereograms]. Klin Monatsbl Augenheilkd 2017. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-118100.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Sood GC, Sen DK. Measurement and diagnosis of cyclodeviation by the after-image method. Br J Ophthalmol 1970;54:340-1.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Herzau V, Joos-Kratsch E. Objective and subjective evaluation of cyclovergence and cyclofusion. Doc Ophthalmol Adv Ophthalmol 1984;58:85-90.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Simonsz HJ, Crone RA, de Waal BJ, Schooneman M, Lorentz de Haas HA. Measurement of the mechanical stiffness in cyclotorsion of the human eye. Vision Res 1984;24:961-7.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Paris V. [Advantages of Krats's laser pointer associated with the Weiss coordimeter in the study of cyclofusion]. J Fr Ophtalmol 2005;28:1081-8.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Prakash G, Choudhary V, Sharma N, Titiyal JS. Change in the accommodative convergence per unit of accommodation ratio after bilateral laser in situ keratomileusis for myopia in orthotropic patients: Prospective evaluation. J Cataract Refract Surg 2007;33:2054-6.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Han J, Hong S, Lee S, Kim JK, Lee HK, Han S-H. Changes in fusional vergence amplitudes after laser refractive surgery for moderate myopia. J Cataract Refract Surg 2014;40:1670-5.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Guyton DL. Ocular torsion: Sensorimotor principles. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 1988;226:241-5.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Bradley A, Robin J, Freeman RD. Nonoptical determinants of Anisekonia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1983;24:507-12.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Lyle TK, Wybar KC. Lyle and Jackson's Practical Orthoptics in the Treatment of Squint. 5th ed. London: H. K Lewis and Co. Ltd; 1967.  Back to cited text no. 19
    


    Figures

  [Figure 1], [Figure 2]
 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
Abstract
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
References
Article Figures
Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed216    
    Printed0    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded26    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal